亚洲精品一区-91视频免费在线观看-老司机精品福利视频-日本国产一区-国产黄色精品-久久久久久亚洲精品

雅思聽力section 4真題:蜜蜂的交流

雕龍文庫 分享 時間: 收藏本文

雅思聽力section 4真題:蜜蜂的交流

  Section 4

  A paper published in Nature on May 12th provides new data that resolves a long-standing scientific controversy. In the 1960s, Nobel Prize winning zoologist, Karl von Frisch, proposed that honeybees use dance as a coded message to guide other bees to new food sources. However, some scientists did not accept von Frischs theory. Using harmonic radar, scientists, funded in part by the UKs Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council have now tracked the flight of bees that had attended a waggle dance and found that they flew straight to the vicinity of the feeding site, as predicted by von Frisch. The tracks allowed the scientists to determine how accurately bees translate the dance code into successful navigation, and showed that they correct for wind drift even when en route to destinations they have never visited before.

  If a honeybee worker discovers a good feeding site it is believed that she informs her nest mates through a dance that describes the distance and direction of the feeding site. This dance language was first described by Karl von Frisch in the 1960s but his experiments also showed that bees that had attended the dance took far longer to get to food than would be expected. This time delay caused other scientists to argue that the recruits did not read the abstract code in the dance at all, but found the food source simply by tracking down the smell that they had picked up from the dancing bee. Another suggestion was that recruits simply followed the dancer when she flew back to the food, and then other bees joined in. The controversy has persisted because prior to the advent of harmonic radar, no one could show exactly where the recruits flew when they left their hives.

  The scientists watched the waggle dance occurring in a glass observation hive and identified recruits. They captured these recruits as they left the hive, attached a radar transponder to them and then tracked their flight paths using harmonic radar. Most recruited bees undertook a flight path that took them straight to the vicinity of the feeding site where they all spent a lot of time in searching flights, trying to locate its exact position. This searching behaviour accounts for the time lag that caused the original controversy.

  In another set of experiments, bee recruits leaving the hive were taken to release sites up to 250m away. These bees flew, not to the feeding site, but in the direction that would have taken them to the feeding site had they not been displaced from the hive. This result adds weight to von Frischs original theory and allows alternative hypotheses about bee behaviour to be firmly discounted.

  Entomologists have long known that bees use polarized sunlight to navigate. Two Swiss scientists now say that a bees navigational map lies embedded in special photoreceptors in its eyes. According to Samuel Rossel and Rudiger Wehner of the University of Zurich, ... the array of receptors forms a template which the bee uses to scan and match the polarization patterns in the sky.

  In the 1940s, Nobel laureate Karl von Frisch showed that bees have a simple yet elegant way of communicating the location of distant sources of food. When a foraging bee returns to the hive, she performs a waggle dance consisting of a short run ending in a loop that returns her to the beginning point of her run. The direction of her run indicates the direction of the food source with respect to the sun.

  A sister bee observing this performance somehow remembers the size of the angle between the sun and the food indicated by the dancing bee. She flies out of the hive, makes a quick calculation of the position of the sun, and zips away at the same angle.

  Bees have compound eyes made of almost 6,000 tiny lenses covering the openings of equally tiny tubes. Each tube contains eight light receptors that look like toothbrushes with the bristles facing each other at the lens end of the tube; the handle is the nerve going to the brane. The tubes located at the top of the bees eye contain toothbrushes that specialize in detecting polarized ultraviolet light. Beginning at the back of the bees compound eye and continuing around to the front, these specialized photoreceptors in each tube are arranged in a pattern that matches the direction of polarized sunlight.

  Polarization results when the atmosphere scatters incoming sunlight and restricts the lights electrical field to a certain direction. When polarized sunlight enters a bees eye, it stimulates the bristles, which in turn stimulate the photoreceptor handles that send a message to the bees brain. Polarized sunlight with an electrical field direction that matches the direction of the bristles stimulates the bees eye more than any other type of light.

  In a complicated series of experiments described in the Sept. 11 NATURE, Rossel and Wehner showed that a bee flies in a circle until the special receptors in her eye detect the maximum stimulation from polarized light. The map in her eye tells her that, in this position, she is facing directly away from the sun. Remembering the orientation of her sister bees waggle dance back at the hive, the bee veers off at the same angle to make a beeline for lunch.

  

  Section 4

  A paper published in Nature on May 12th provides new data that resolves a long-standing scientific controversy. In the 1960s, Nobel Prize winning zoologist, Karl von Frisch, proposed that honeybees use dance as a coded message to guide other bees to new food sources. However, some scientists did not accept von Frischs theory. Using harmonic radar, scientists, funded in part by the UKs Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council have now tracked the flight of bees that had attended a waggle dance and found that they flew straight to the vicinity of the feeding site, as predicted by von Frisch. The tracks allowed the scientists to determine how accurately bees translate the dance code into successful navigation, and showed that they correct for wind drift even when en route to destinations they have never visited before.

  If a honeybee worker discovers a good feeding site it is believed that she informs her nest mates through a dance that describes the distance and direction of the feeding site. This dance language was first described by Karl von Frisch in the 1960s but his experiments also showed that bees that had attended the dance took far longer to get to food than would be expected. This time delay caused other scientists to argue that the recruits did not read the abstract code in the dance at all, but found the food source simply by tracking down the smell that they had picked up from the dancing bee. Another suggestion was that recruits simply followed the dancer when she flew back to the food, and then other bees joined in. The controversy has persisted because prior to the advent of harmonic radar, no one could show exactly where the recruits flew when they left their hives.

  The scientists watched the waggle dance occurring in a glass observation hive and identified recruits. They captured these recruits as they left the hive, attached a radar transponder to them and then tracked their flight paths using harmonic radar. Most recruited bees undertook a flight path that took them straight to the vicinity of the feeding site where they all spent a lot of time in searching flights, trying to locate its exact position. This searching behaviour accounts for the time lag that caused the original controversy.

  In another set of experiments, bee recruits leaving the hive were taken to release sites up to 250m away. These bees flew, not to the feeding site, but in the direction that would have taken them to the feeding site had they not been displaced from the hive. This result adds weight to von Frischs original theory and allows alternative hypotheses about bee behaviour to be firmly discounted.

  Entomologists have long known that bees use polarized sunlight to navigate. Two Swiss scientists now say that a bees navigational map lies embedded in special photoreceptors in its eyes. According to Samuel Rossel and Rudiger Wehner of the University of Zurich, ... the array of receptors forms a template which the bee uses to scan and match the polarization patterns in the sky.

  In the 1940s, Nobel laureate Karl von Frisch showed that bees have a simple yet elegant way of communicating the location of distant sources of food. When a foraging bee returns to the hive, she performs a waggle dance consisting of a short run ending in a loop that returns her to the beginning point of her run. The direction of her run indicates the direction of the food source with respect to the sun.

  A sister bee observing this performance somehow remembers the size of the angle between the sun and the food indicated by the dancing bee. She flies out of the hive, makes a quick calculation of the position of the sun, and zips away at the same angle.

  Bees have compound eyes made of almost 6,000 tiny lenses covering the openings of equally tiny tubes. Each tube contains eight light receptors that look like toothbrushes with the bristles facing each other at the lens end of the tube; the handle is the nerve going to the brane. The tubes located at the top of the bees eye contain toothbrushes that specialize in detecting polarized ultraviolet light. Beginning at the back of the bees compound eye and continuing around to the front, these specialized photoreceptors in each tube are arranged in a pattern that matches the direction of polarized sunlight.

  Polarization results when the atmosphere scatters incoming sunlight and restricts the lights electrical field to a certain direction. When polarized sunlight enters a bees eye, it stimulates the bristles, which in turn stimulate the photoreceptor handles that send a message to the bees brain. Polarized sunlight with an electrical field direction that matches the direction of the bristles stimulates the bees eye more than any other type of light.

  In a complicated series of experiments described in the Sept. 11 NATURE, Rossel and Wehner showed that a bee flies in a circle until the special receptors in her eye detect the maximum stimulation from polarized light. The map in her eye tells her that, in this position, she is facing directly away from the sun. Remembering the orientation of her sister bees waggle dance back at the hive, the bee veers off at the same angle to make a beeline for lunch.

  

主站蜘蛛池模板: 亚洲欧美日韩国产一区二区精品 | 亚洲一级在线 | 亚洲精品无码不卡在线播放he | 高清午夜毛片 | 亚洲精品一区二区久久 | 麻豆影音 | 暖暖视频日韩欧美在线观看 | 最新福利片v国产片 | 国产日韩欧美一区二区 | 九月婷婷亚洲综合在线 | a国产成人免费视频 | 国产成人午夜福在线观看 | 欧美笫一页| 国产成人精品免费视频大全软件 | 色老久久| 国产精品一区二区资源 | 美女视频黄a全部 | 亚洲成人偷拍自拍 | 国产欧美日韩成人 | 国产性做久久久久久 | 久视频免费精品6 | 亚洲乱码国产一区网址 | 97久久精品一区二区三区 | 欧美三级真做在线观看 | 性生活视频网站 | 亚洲精品久久久久综合91 | 日本免费久久 | 久久成年视频 | 麻豆19禁国产青草精品 | 中国美女黄色一级片 | 成人a毛片免费视频观看 | 亚洲欧美另类自拍第一页 | 国产短视频精品一区二区三区 | 国产精品国产亚洲精品不卡 | 日韩免费一区二区三区在线 | 亚洲精品一区二区三区四区 | 国产欧美日韩在线一区二区不卡 | 美女张开腿双腿让男人桶 | 黄色毛片播放 | 久久精品成人一区二区三区 | 免费国产a理论片 |